A Theoretical Proposal for a Perceptually Driven, Food-Based Disgust that Can Influence Food Acceptance During Early Childhood

Authors

  • Steven Daniel Brown Department of Psychology, University of Derby, Kedleston Rd, DE22 1GB, UK
  • Gillian Harris School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, B15 2TT, UK

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.6000/1929-4247.2012.01.01.01

Keywords:

Disgust, Infant/child eating behaviours, Food neophobia, Contagion, Picky eating

Abstract

Disgust, the “revulsion at the prospect of (oral) incorporation of an offensive substance”, is not thought to influence the acceptability of food during infancy and early childhood. This is because the feelings of disgust require a person to have developed an understanding of contagion and to be aware of the nature and origin of a given disgust stimulus, which does not occur until around seven years of age. Despite this need for higher cognitive functioning, studies have demonstrated the potential for disgust in children as young as two years of age. Furthermore, it seems that young children can demonstrate aspects of disgust without having the cognitive understanding of contagion. This review is the first paper to demonstrate how core disgust may influence the acceptability of foods from late infancy. Firstly, food neophobia may act as a catalyst for disgust. Secondly, that disgust in young children can result from the visual perceptual features of food (as opposed to a cognitive response based on non-food disgust stimuli). Thirdly, that some disliked foods have contaminating properties, much like non-food, adult disgust stimuli (e.g. insects). Fourthly, that the response reduces as the child ages and learns more about food and its variability between presentations. Finally, individual differences exist to explain why an individual child may be more or less likely to respond to a given food with a disgust response. This proposal adds to the current debate relating to the motivations of ‘picky’ eating during early childhood and introduces an alternative to the proposal that these behaviours are the result of a child’s desire for autonomy.

 

References

Addessi E, Galloway AT, Visalberghi E, Birch LL. Specific social influences on the acceptance of novel foods in 2-5-year-old children. Appetite 2005; 45(3): 264-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.007

Cashdan E. Adaptiveness of food learning and food aversions in children. Social Sci Inform 1998; 37(4): 613-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/053901898037004003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/053901898037004003

Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite 2003; 41(2): 205-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00048-5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00048-5

Harper LV, Sanders KM. The effect of adults’ eating on young children’s acceptance of unfamiliar foods. J Exper Child Psychol 1975; 20(2): 206-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(75)90098-3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(75)90098-3

Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba BC, Pirok E, Steinberg L. What kind of exposure reduces children’s food neophobia? Looking vs. tasting. Appetite 1987; 9(3): 171-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(87)80011-9 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(87)80011-9

Koivisto UK, Sjödén PO. Food and general neophobia in Swedish families: parent-child comparisons and relationships with serving specific foods. Appetite 1996; 26(2): 107-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1996.0009 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1996.0009

Pelchat ML, Pliner P. “Try it. You’ll like it.” Effects of information on willingness to try novel foods. Appetite 1995; 24(2): 153-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(95)99373-8 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(95)99373-8

Rozin P. The significance of learning mechanisms in food selection. In: Barker L, Best M, Domjan M, Eds. Learning mechanisms in food selection: Some biology, psychology, and sociology of science. Houston, Texas: Baylor University press 1977; pp. 557-92.

Cooke LJ, Haworth CM, Wardle J. Genetic and environmental influences on children’s food neophobia. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 86(2): 428-33. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/86.2.428

Birch LL, Marlin DW. I don’t like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old children’s food preferences. Appetite 1982; 3(4): 353-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80053-6 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80053-6

Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K, Laing D. Infants’ Consumption of a New Food Enhances Acceptance of Similar Foods. Appetite 1998; 30(3): 283-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1997.0146 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1997.0146

Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite 2005; 44(3): 289-97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.01.006 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.01.006

Pliner P. The effects of mere exposure on liking for edible substances. Appetite 1982; 3(3): 283-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80026-3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80026-3

Wardle J, Herrera M-L, Cooke L, Gibson EL. Modifying children’s food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 2003; 57(2): 341-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601541 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601541

Wardle J, Cooke LJ, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M. Increasing children’s acceptance of vegetables; a randomized trial of parent-led exposure. Appetite 2003; 40(2): 155-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(02)00135-6 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(02)00135-6

Birch LL. Effects of Peer Models’ Food Choices and Eating Behaviors on Preschoolers’ Food Preferences. Child Dev 1980; 51(2): 489-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129283 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1980.tb02570.x

Hendy HM. Effectiveness of trained peer models to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite 2002; 39(3): 217-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.2002.0510 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2002.0510

Hendy HM, Raudenbush B. Effectiveness of teacher modeling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite 2000; 34(1): 61-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1999.0286 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1999.0286

Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI. Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers’ decisions about offering a new food. J Am Dietet Assoc 2004; 104(Suppl 1): 57-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2003.10.024 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2003.10.024

Brown SD, Harris G. Disliked food acting as a contaminant during infancy. A disgust based motivation for rejection. Appetite 2012; 58(2): 535-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.010 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.010

Stevenson RJ, Oaten MJ, Case TI, Repacholi BM, Wagland P. Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: development and acquisition. Dev Psychol 2010; 46(1): 165-77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016692 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016692

Toyama N. Young children’s awareness of socially mediated rejection of food: Why is food dropped at the table “dirty”? Cogn Dev 2000; 15(4): 523-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00041-7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00041-7

Rozin P, Haidt J, McCauley C. Disgust. 3rd ed. Lewis M, Haviland-Jones J, Barrett L, Ed. New York: Guilford Press 2008.

Fallon AE, Rozin P. The psychological bases of food rejections by humans. Ecol Food Nutr 1983; 13(1): 15-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03670244.1983.9990728 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/03670244.1983.9990728

Rozin P, Fallon AE. A perspective on disgust. Psychol Rev 1987; 94(1): 23-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.94.1.23

Logue AW, Smith ME. Predictors of food preferences in adult humans. Appetite 1986; 7(2): 109-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(86)80012-5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(86)80012-5

Rozin P. Development in the Food Domain. Dev Psychol 1990; 26(4): 555-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.555 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.26.4.555

Angyal A. Disgust and related aversions. J Abnor Soc Psychol 1941; 36(3): 393-12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0058254 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0058254

Curtis V, Biran A. Dirt, disgust, and disease. Is hygiene in our genes? Perspect Biol Med 2001; 44(1): 17-31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/pbm.2001.0001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1353/pbm.2001.0001

Darwin C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals: Definitive Edition. 200th Anniversary edition (Reissue). Ekman P, Ed. Harper Perennial 2009.

Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev 1988; 59(6): 1555-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130670 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03683.x

Rozin P, Lowery L, Ebert R. Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994; 66(5): 870-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.870 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.66.5.870

Steiner JE, Glaser D, Hawilo ME, Berridge KC. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2001; 25(1): 53-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00051-8 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00051-8

Vrana SR. The psychophysiology of disgust: differentiating negative emotional contexts with facial EMG. Psychophysiology 1993; 30(3): 279-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03354.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03354.x

Bennett DS, Bendersky M, Lewis M. Does the Organization of Emotional Expression Change Over Time? Facial Expressivity From 4 to 12 Months. Infancy 2005; 8(2): 167-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0802_4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0802_4

Sullivan M, Lewis M. Emotional Expressions of Young Infants and Children: A Practitioner’s primer. Infants Young Children 2003; 16(2): 120-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200304000-00005 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200304000-00005

Fallon AE, Rozin P, Pliner P. The child’s conception of food: the development of food rejections with special reference to disgust and contamination sensitivity. Child Dev 1984; 55(2): 566-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129968 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00317.x

Toronchuk J, Ellis G. Disgust: Sensory affect or primary emotional system? Cogn Emot 2007; 21(8): 1799-818. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930701298515 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701298515

Miller WI. The Anatomy of Disgust. New Ed. Harvard University Press 1998. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctv26071fj

Rosen AB, Rozin P. Now You See It, Now You Don’t: The Preschool Child’s Conception of Invisible Particles in the Context of Dissolving. Dev Psychol 1993; 29(2): 300-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.2.300 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.29.2.300

Siegal M. Children’s knowledge of contagion and contamination as causes of illness. Child Dev 1988; 59(5): 1353-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130497 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01503.x

Springer K, Belk A. The Role of Physical Contact and Association in Early Contamination Sensitivity. Dev Psychol 1994; 30(6): 864-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.864 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.30.6.864

Toyama N. Developmental Changes in the Basis of Associational Contamination Thinking. Cogn Dev 1999; 14(2): 343-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00008-8 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00008-8

Rozin P, Hammer L, Oster H, Horowitz T, Marmora V. The child’s conception of food: differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range. Appetite 1986; 7(2): 141-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(86)80014-9 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(86)80014-9

Carruth BR, Skinner J, Houck K, Moran J, Coletta F, Ott D. The Phenomenon of “Picky Eater”: A Behavioral Marker in Eating Patterns of Toddlers. J Am Coll Nutr 1998; 17(2): 180-86. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/07315724.1998.10718744

Kim YJ, Chung SJ, Han YS, et al. The Characteristics of Infants’ Temperament, Maternal Feeding Behavior and Feeding Practices in Picky Eaters. Korean J Commun Nutr 2005; 10(4): 462-70. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1096/fasebj.20.4.A174

Jacobi C, Agras WS, Bryson S, Hammer LD. Behavioral validation, precursors, and concomitants of picky eating in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2003; 42(1): 76-84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200301000-00013 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200301000-00013

Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL. Predictors and consequences of food neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003; 103(6): 692-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/jada.2003.50134 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1053/jada.2003.50134

Pliner P, Eng A, Krishnan K. The effects of fear and hunger on food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1995; 25(1): 77-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1995.0042 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1995.0042

Pliner P, Hobden K. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1992; 19(2): 105-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(92)90014-W DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(92)90014-W

Pliner P, Pelchat M, Grabski M. Reduction of neophobia in humans by exposure to novel foods. Appetite 1993; 20(2): 111-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1013 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1013

Singer LT, Ambuel B, Wade S, Jaffe AC. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of health-impairing food phobias in children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1992; 31(5): 847-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199209000-00011 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199209000-00011

Nicholls D, Christie D, Randall L, Lask B. Selective Eating: Symptom, Disorder or Normal Variant. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 6(2): 257-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359104501006002007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104501006002007

Cisler JM, Olatunji BO, Lohr JM. Disgust, fear, and the anxiety disorders: A critical review. Clin Psychol Rev 2009; 29(1): 34-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.007

de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H. Blood-injection-injury phobia and fear of spiders: Domain specific individual differences in disgust sensitivity. Personality Individual Differen 1998; 24(2): 153-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00178-5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00178-5

Mulkens SA, de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H. Disgust and spider phobia. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105(3): 464-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.464 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843X.105.3.464

Tolin DF, Lohr JM, Sawchuk CN, Lee TC. Disgust and disgust sensitivity in blood-injection-injury and spider phobia. Behav Res Ther 1997; 35(10): 949-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00048-X DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00048-X

Thorpe SJ, Salkovskis PM. Studies on the role of disgust in the acquisition and maintenance of specific phobias. Behav Res Ther 1998; 36(9): 877-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00066-7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00066-7

Martins Y, Pliner P. “Ugh! That’s disgusting!”: Identification of the characteristics of foods underlying rejections based on disgust. Appetite 2006; 46(1): 75-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.09.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.09.001

Simpson J, Anthony SH, Schmeer S, Overton PG. Food-related contextual factors substantially modify the disgust response. Food Quality Preference 2007; 18(2): 183-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.09.016 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.09.016

Batsell WR Jr, Brown AS, Ansfield ME, Paschall GY. “You will eat all of that!”: a retrospective analysis of forced consumption episodes. Appetite 2002; 38(3): 211-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.2001.0482 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2001.0482

Batsell Jr. WR, Brown AS. Human Flavor-Aversion Learning: A Comparison of Traditional Aversions and Cognitive Aversions. Learn Motivat 1998; 29(4): 383-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1998.1016 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1998.1016

Tulve NS, Suggs JC, McCurdy T, Cohen Hubal EA, Moya J. Frequency of mouthing behavior in young children. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol 2002; 12(4): 259-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.jea.7500225 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.jea.7500225

Garcia J, Quick D, White B. Conditioning disgust and fear from mollusk to monkey. In: Alkon DL, Farley J, Eds. Primary Neural Substrates of Learning and Behavioural Change. CUP Archive 1984.

Domjan M. Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 2005; 56: 179-206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409

Harris G. Developmental, regulatory and cognitive aspects of feeding disorders. In: Southall A, Schwartz A, Eds. Feeding Problems in Children. Radcliffe Publishing 2000.

Timimi S, Douglas J, Tsiftsopoulou K. Selective eaters: a retrospective case note study. Child Care Health Dev 1997; 23(3): 265-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.1997.tb00968.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.1997.tb00968.x

Brown SD, Harris G, Bell L, Lines LM. Disliked food acting as a contaminant in a sample of young children. Appetite 2012; 58(3): 991-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.02.047 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.02.047

Pliner P, Salvy S-J. Food neophobia in humans. In: Shepherd R, Raats M, editors. The Psychology of Food Choice. CABI 2007. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1079/9780851990323.0075

Nguyen SP, Murphy GL. An Apple is More Than Just a Fruit: Cross-Classification in Children’s Concepts. Child Dev 2003; 74(6): 1783-806. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00638.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00638.x

Desor JA, Maller O, Turner RE. Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants. J Compar Physiol Psychol 1973; 84(3): 496-501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034906 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034906

Harris G, Thomas A, Booth DA. Development of salt taste in infancy. Dev Psychol 1990; 26(4): 534-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.534 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.26.4.534

Zellner DA, Siemers E, Teran V, et al. Neatness counts. How plating affects liking for the taste of food. Appetite 2011; 57(3): 642-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.004

Blossfeld I, Collins A, Kiely M, Delahunty C. Texture preferences of 12-month-old infants and the role of early experiences. Food Quality Preference 2007; 18(2): 396-404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2006.03.022 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2006.03.022

Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Delayed introduction of lumpy foods to children during the complementary feeding period affects child’s food acceptance and feeding at 7 years of age. Matern Child Nutr 2009; 5(1): 75-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-8709.2008.00153.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-8709.2008.00153.x

Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler PJ. Children’s Food Preferences: A Longitudinal Analysis. J Am Dietet Assoc 2002; 102(11): 1638-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90349-4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90349-4

Skuse D. Identification and management of problem eaters. Arch Dis Child 1993; 69(5): 604-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.69.5.604 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.69.5.604

Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Infant Dietary Experience and Acceptance of Solid Foods. Pediatrics 1994; 93(2): 271-7. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.93.2.271

Marcontell DK, Laster AE, Johnson J. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of food neophobia in adults. J Anxiety Disord 2003; 17(2): 243-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6185(01)00090-1 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6185(01)00090-1

Dunn W, Brown C. Factor analysis on the Sensory Profile from a national sample of children without disabilities. Am J Occup Ther 1997; 51(7): 490-495; discussion 496-499. http://dx.doi.org/10.5014/ajot.51.7.490 DOI: https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.51.7.490

Schreck KA, Williams K, Smith AF. A comparison of eating behaviors between children with and without autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2004; 34(4): 433-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86

Williams KE, Gibbons BG, Schreck KA. Comparing Selective Eaters with and Without Developmental Disabilities. J Dev Phys Disabilities 2005; 17(3): 299-309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10882-005-4387-7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-005-4387-7

Coulthard H, Blissett J. Fruit and vegetable consumption in children and their mothers. Moderating effects of child sensory sensitivity. Appetite 2009; 52(2): 410-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.015 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.015

Cardello AV. Consumer concerns and expectations about novel food processing technologies: effects on product liking. Appetite 2003; 40(3): 217-33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00008-4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00008-4

McCarthy M. Waitrose offers “ugly” fruit and vegetables at discount rate. The Independent 2006 June 19.

Harris G. Introducing the Infant’s First Solid Food. Br Food J 1993; 95(9): 7-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/00070709310045004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1108/00070709310045004

Galloway AT, Fiorito LM, Francis LA, Birch LL. “Finish your soup”: counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite 2006; 46(3): 318-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.01.019 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.01.019

Farrow C, Blissett J. Maternal cognitions, psychopathologic symptoms, and infant temperament as predictors of early infant feeding problems: a longitudinal study. Int J Eat Disord 2006; 39(2): 128-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20220 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20220

Hughes SO, Power TG, Orlet Fisher J, Mueller S, Nicklas TA. Revisiting a neglected construct: parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite 2005; 44(1): 83-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.007

Patrick H, Nicklas TA, Hughes SO, Morales M. The benefits of authoritative feeding style: caregiver feeding styles and children’s food consumption patterns. Appetite 2005; 44(2): 243-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2002.07.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2002.07.001

Downloads

Published

2012-09-26 — Updated on 2022-11-25

Versions

How to Cite

Brown, S. D., & Harris, G. (2022). A Theoretical Proposal for a Perceptually Driven, Food-Based Disgust that Can Influence Food Acceptance During Early Childhood. International Journal of Child Health and Nutrition, 1(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.6000/1929-4247.2012.01.01.01 (Original work published September 26, 2012)

Issue

Section

General Articles